References
Abrahms, B., Hazen, E.L., Aikens, E.O., Savoca, M.S., Goldbogen, J.A.,
Bograd, S.J., Jacox, M.G., Irvine, L.M., Palacios, D.M. and Mate, B.R.,
2019. Memory and resource tracking drive blue whale
migrations. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences , 116 (12), pp.5582-5587.
Alerstam,
T., Hedenstrom, A. & Akesson, S. (2003). Long-distance migration:
evolution and determinants. Oikos , 103, 247–260.
Allred, B.W., Fuhlendorf, S.D., Engle, D.M. & Elmore, R.D. (2011).
Ungulate preference for burned patches reveals strength: Of fire-grazing
interaction. Ecol. Evol. , 1, 132–144.
Arditi, R. & Dacorogna, B. (1988). Optimal foraging on arbitrary food
distributions and the definition of habitat patches. Am. Nat. ,
131, 837–846.
Auger-Méthé, M., Field, C., Albertsen, C.M., Derocher, A.E., Lewis,
M.A., Jonsen, I.D., et al. (2016). State-space models’ dirty
little secrets: Even simple linear Gaussian models can have estimation
problems. Sci. Rep. , 6, 1–10.
Avgar, T., Deardon, R. & Fryxell, J.M. (2013). An empirically
parameterized individual based model of animal movement, perception, and
memory. Ecol. Modell. , 251, 158–172.
Avgar, T., Potts, J.R., Lewis, M.A. & Boyce, M.S. (2016). Integrated
step selection analysis: Bridging the gap between resource selection and
animal movement. Methods Ecol. Evol. , 7, 619–630.
Bacon, I.E., Hurly, T.A. and Healy, S., 2010. Both the past and the
present affect risk-sensitive decisions of foraging rufous
hummingbirds. Behavioral Ecology , 21 (3), pp.626-632.
Bandura, A. & Walters, R.H. (1963). Social learning and
personality development . International Thomson Publishing.
Barry, T., Gurarie, E., Cheraghi, F., Kojola, I. & Fagan, W.F. (In
Press) Does dispersal make the heart grow bolder? Avoidance of
anthropogenic habitat elements across wolf life history. Animal
Behaviour .
Bartlam-Brooks, H.L.A., Beck, P.S.A., Bohrer, G. & Harris, S. (2013).
In search of greener pastures: Using satellite images to predict the
effects of environmental change on zebra migration. J. Geophys.
Res. Biogeosciences , 118, 1427–1437.
Bejder, L., Samuels, A., Whitehead, H., Finn, H. & Allen, S. (2009).
Impact assessment research: Use and misuse of habituation, sensitisation
and tolerance in describing wildlife responses to anthropogenic stimuli.Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. , 395, 177–185.
Berbert, J.M. & Lewis, M.A. (2018). Superdiffusivity due to resource
depletion in random searches. Ecol. Complex. , 33, 41–48.
Berger-Tal, O., Nathan, J., Meron, E., & Saltz, D. (2014). The
exploration-exploitation dilemma: a multidisciplinary framework.PloS One , 9(4).
Berger-Tal, O. and Saltz, D., 2014. Using the movement patterns of
reintroduced animals to improve reintroduction success. Current
Zoology , 60 (4), pp.515-526.
Bingman, V.P., Gagliardo, A., Hough, G.E., Ioalé, P., Kahn, M.C. and
Siegel, J.J., 2005. The avian hippocampus, homing in pigeons and the
memory representation of large-scale space. Integrative and Comparative
Biology, 45(3), pp.555-564.
Biro, D., Freeman, R., Meade, J., Roberts, S. and Guilford, T., 2007.
Pigeons combine compass and landmark guidance in familiar route
navigation. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences , 104 (18), pp.7471-7476.
Bischof, R., Loe, L.E., Meisingset, E.L., Zimmermann, B., van Moorter,
B. & Mysterud, A. (2012). A migratory northern ungulate in the pursuit
of spring: Jumping or surfing the green wave? Am. Nat. , 180,
407–424.
Bograd, S.J., Block, B.A., Costa, D.P. and Godley, B.J., 2010.
Biologging technologies: new tools for conservation.
Introduction. Endangered Species Research , 10 , pp.1-7.
Bowell, R.J., Warren, A. & Redmond, I. (1996). Formation of cave salts
and utilization by elephants in the Mount Elgon region, Kenya.Geol. Soc. London, Spec. Publ. , 113, 63–79.
Boyer, D., & Walsh, P. D. (2010). Modelling the mobility of living
organisms in heterogeneous landscapes: does memory improve foraging
success? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society A ,
368(1933), 5645-5659.
Bracis, C., Gurarie, E., Van Moorter, B. & Goodwin, R.A. (2015). Memory
effects on movement behavior in animal foraging. PLoS One , 10,
1–21.
Bracis, C., Gurarie, E., Rutter, J.D. & Goodwin, R.A. (2018).
Remembering the good and the bad: memory-based mediation of the
food–safety trade-off in dynamic landscapes. Theor. Ecol. , 11,
305–319.
Bracis, C. & Mueller, T. (2017). Memory, not just perception, plays an
important role in terrestrial mammalian migration. Proc. R. Soc. B
Biol. Sci. , 284.
Brown, J.S. (1988). Patch use as an indicator of habitat preference,
predation risk, and competition. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. , 22,
37–47.
Brown, C., 2001. Familiarity with the test environment improves escape
responses in the crimson spotted rainbowfish, Melanotaenia
duboulayi . Animal Cognition , 4 (2), pp.109-113.
Brown, C.R., Brown, M.B. and Brazeal, K.R., 2008. Familiarity with
breeding habitat improves daily survival in colonial cliff
swallows. Animal Behaviour , 76 (4), pp.1201-1210.
Charnov, E.L. (1976). Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem.Theor. Popul. Biol. , 9, 129–136.
Chater, N., Tenenbaum, J.B. & Yuille, A. (2006). Probabilistic models
of cognition: Conceptual foundations. Trends Cogn. Sci. , 10,
287–291.
Cheptou, P. O., A. L. Hargreaves, D. Bonte, and H. Jacquemyn. 2017.
“Adaptation to fragmentation: Evolutionary dynamics driven by human
influences.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:
Biological Sciences 372 (1712).
Clarke, M.F., K.B. da Silva, H. Lair, R. Pocklington, D.L. Kramer and
R.L. McLaughlin. (1993). Site familiarity affects escape behaviour of
the eastern chipmunk, Tamias striatus . Oikos , 66,
533–537.
Colbeck, G.J., Duchesne, P., Postma, L.D., Lesage, V., Hammill, M.O. &
Turgeon, J. (2013). Groups of related belugas (Delphinapterus
leucas ) travel together during their seasonal migrations in and around
Hudson Bay. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. , 280.
Dall, S.R.X., Giraldeau, L.A., Olsson, O., McNamara, J.M. & Stephens,
D.W. (2005). Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology.Trends Ecol. Evol. , 20, 187–193.
Dolev, A., Saltz, D., Bar-David, S. & Yom-Tov, Y. (2002). Impact of
repeated releases on space-use patterns of Persian fallow deer. J.
Wildl. Manage. , 66, 737.
Doll, B. B., Simon, D. A., & Daw, N. D. (2012). The ubiquity of
model-based reinforcement learning. Current opinion in neurobiology,
22(6), 1075-1081.
Dorrance, B.R. & Zentall, T.R. (2001). Imitative learning in Japanese
quail (Coturnix japonica ) depends on the motivational state of
the observer quail at the time of observation. J. Comp. Psychol. ,
115, 62–67.
Dunlap, A.S. and Stephens, D.W., 2009. Components of change in the
evolution of learning and unlearned preference. Proceedings of the
Royal Society B: Biological Sciences , 276 (1670), pp.3201-3208.
Dupuis-Desormeaux, M., Kaaria, T.N., Mwololo, M., Davidson, Z. &
MacDonald, S.E. (2018). A ghost fence-gap: Surprising wildlife usage of
an obsolete fence crossing. PeerJ , 2018, 1–18.
Eliassen, S., Jørgensen, C., Mangel, M. & Giske, J. (2007). Exploration
or exploitation: Life expectancy changes the value of learning in
foraging strategies. Oikos , 116, 513–523.
Fagan, W.F., Lewis, M.A., Auger‐Méthé, M., Avgar, T., Benhamou, S.,
Breed, G., LaDage, L., Schlägel, U.E., Tang, W.W., Papastamatiou, Y.P.
and Forester, J., 2013. Spatial memory and animal movement. Ecology
Letters 16: 1316-1329.
Fishlock, V., Caldwell, C. & Lee, P.C. (2016). Elephant resource-use
traditions. Anim. Cogn. , 19, 429–433.
Flores-Abreu, I.N., Hurly, T.A. & Healy, S.D. (2012). One-trial spatial
learning: Wild hummingbirds relocate a reward after a single visit.Anim. Cogn. , 15, 631–637.
Ford, A.T., Goheen, J.R., Augustine, D.J., Kinnaird, M.F., O’Brien,
T.G., Palmer, T.M., Pringle, R.M., Woodroffe, R., 2015. Recovery of
African wild dogs suppresses prey but does not trigger a trophic
cascade. Ecology 96, 2705–2714.
Ford, A.T. and Clevenger, A.P., 2018. Factors affecting the permeability
of road mitigation measures to the movement of small
mammals. Canadian Journal of Zoology , 97 (4), pp.379-384.
Fortin, D., Beyer, H.L., Boyce, M.S., Smith, D.W., Duchesne, T. & Mao,
J.S. (2005). Wolves influence elk movements: Behavior shapes a trophic
cascade in Yellowstone National Park. Ecology , 86, 1320–1330.
Fragaszy, D. & Visalberghi, E. (2004). Socially biased learning in
monkeys. Learn. Behav. , 32, 24–35.
Frair, J.L., Merrill, E.H., Allen, J.R. & Boyce, M.S. (2007). Know thy
enemy: Experience affects elk translocation success in risky landscapes.J. Wildl. Manage. , 71, 541–554.
Franklin, E.L. and Franks, N.R., 2012. Individual and social learning in
tandem-running recruitment by ants. Animal
behaviour , 84 (2), pp.361-368.
Franks, N.R., Hooper, J.W., Dornhaus, A., Aukett, P.J., Hayward, A.L. &
Berghoff, S.M. (2007). Reconnaissance and latent learning in ants.Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. , 274, 1505–1509.
Franks, N.R. & Richardson, T. (2006). Teaching in tandem-running ants.Nature , 439, 153–153.
Fryxell, J.M. & Sinclair, A.R.E. (1988). Causes and consequences of
migration by large herbivores. Trends Ecol. Evol. , 3, 237–241.
Fryxell, J.M., Wilmshurst, J.F. & Sinclair, A.R.E. (2004). Predictive
models of movement by serengeti grazers. Ecology , 85, 2429–2435.
Fryxell, J.M., Hazell, M., Börger, L., Dalziel, B.D., Haydon, D.T.,
Morales, J.M., McIntosh, T. and Rosatte, R.C., 2008. Multiple movement
modes by large herbivores at multiple spatiotemporal
scales. Proceedings of the National academy of
Sciences , 105 (49), pp.19114-19119.
Giraldeau, L.A., Valone, T.J. and Templeton, J.J., 2002. Potential
disadvantages of using socially acquired
information. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of
London. Series B: Biological Sciences , 357 (1427), pp.1559-1566.
Grecian, W.J., Lane, J. V., Michelot, T., Wade, H.M. & Hamer, K.C.
(2018). Understanding the ontogeny of foraging behaviour: Insights from
combining marine predator bio-logging with satellite-derived
oceanography in hidden Markov models. J. R. Soc. Interface , 15.
Green, R.F. (1980). Bayesian birds: A simple example of Oaten’s
stochastic model of optimal foraging. Theor. Popul. Biol. , 18,
244–256.
de Grissac, S., Bartumeus, F., Cox, S.L. & Weimerskirch, H. (2017).
Early-life foraging: Behavioral responses of newly fledged albatrosses
to environmental conditions. Ecol. Evol. , 7, 6766–6778.
Gros, C. (2015). Complex and Adaptive Dynamical Systems. Springer.
Gumbert, A. (2000). Color choices by bumble bees (Bombus
terrestris ): Innate preferences and generalization after learning.Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. , 48, 36–43.
Gurarie, E., Suutarinen, J., Kojola, I. & Ovaskainen, O. (2011). Summer
movements, predation and habitat use of wolves in human modified boreal
forests. Oecologia , 165, 891–903.
Hamilton, P.K. & Cooper, L.A. (2010). Changes in North Atlantic right
whale (Eubalaena glacialis ) cow-calf association times and use of
the calving ground: 1993-2005. Mar. Mammal Sci. , 26, 896–916.
Hastie, T., Tibshirani, R. and Friedman, J., 2009. The elements of
statistical learning: prediction, inference and data
mining. Springer-Verlag, New York .
He, K., Dai, Q., Foss-Grant, A., Gurarie, E., Fagan, W.F., Lewis, M.A.,et al. (2019). Movement and activity of reintroduced giant
pandas. Ursus , 29, 163.
Healy, S.D. & Hurly, T.A. (1995). Spatial memory in rufous hummingbirds
(Selasphorus rufus ): A field test. Anim. Learn. Behav. ,
23, 63–68.
Helfman, G.S. & Schultz, E.T. (1984). Social transmission of
behavioural traditions in a coral reef fish. Anim. Behav. , 32,
379–384.
Henderson, J., Hurly, T.A. & Healy, S.D. (2006). Spatial relational
learning in rufous hummingbirds (Selasphorus rufus). Anim. Cogn. ,
9, 201–205.
Heyes, C.M. (1994). Social learning in animals: Categories and
mechanisms. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. , 69, 207–231.
Hoare, R. E., and J.T. Du Toit. 1999. Coexistence between people and
elephants in African savannas.” Conservation Biology 13 (3):
633–39
Holdo, R.M., Holt, R.D. & Fryxell, J.M. (2009). Opposing rainfall and
plant nutritional gradients best explain the wildebeest migration in the
serengeti. Am. Nat. , 173, 431–445.
Huber, R. and M. Knaden. (2018). Desert ants possess distinct memories
for food and nest odors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. , 115,
10470–10474.
Hurme, E., Gurarie, E., Greif, S., Flores-Martínez, J.J., Wilkinson,
G.S. and Yovel, Y., 2019. Acoustic evaluation of behavioral states
predicted from GPS tracking: a case study of a marine fishing bat.Movement ecology , 7 (1), p.21.
Ishii, Y. & Shimada, M. (2010). The effect of learning and search
images on predator-prey interactions. Popul. Ecol. , 52, 27–35.
Jacobs, L.F. & Menzel, R. (2014). Navigation outside of the box: What
the lab can learn from the field and what the field can learn from the
lab. Mov. Ecol. , 2, 1–22.
Janson, C.H. & Byrne, R. (2007). What wild primates know about
resources: Opening up the black box. Anim. Cogn. , 10, 357–367.
Jesmer, B.R., Merkle, J.A., Goheen, J.R., Aikens, E.O., Beck, J.L.,
Courtemanch, A.B., et al. (2018). Is ungulate migration
culturally transmitted? Evidence of social learning from translocated
animals. Science (80-. ). , 361, 1023–1025.
Jordan, M.I. & Mitchell, T.M. (2015). Machine learning: Trends,
perspectives, and prospects. Science (80-. ). , 349, 255–260.
Kays, R., Crofoot, M.C., Jetz, W. and Wikelski, M., 2015. Terrestrial
animal tracking as an eye on life and
planet. Science , 348 (6240), p.aaa2478.
Kendal, R.L., Boogert, N.J., Rendell, L., Laland, K.N., Webster, M. &
Jones, P.L. (2018). Social learning strategies: Bridge-building between
fields. Trends Cogn. Sci. , 22, 651–665.
King, L.E., Douglas-Hamilton, I. & Vollrath, F. (2011). Beehive fences
as effective deterrents for crop-raiding elephants: Field trials in
northern Kenya. Afr. J. Ecol. , 49, 431–439.
Kokko, H., López-Sepulcre, A. and Morrell, L.J., 2006. From hawks and
doves to self-consistent games of territorial behavior. The
American Naturalist , 167 (6), pp.901-912.
Kloppers, E.L., St. Clair, C.C. & Hurd, T.E. (2005).
Predator-resembling aversive conditioning for managing habituated
wildlife. Ecol. Soc. , 10.
Krebs, J.R. & Inman, A.J. (1992). Learning and foraging: Individuals,
groups, and populations. Am. Nat. , 140, S63–S84.
Kubandt, F., Nowak, M., Koglin, T., Gros, C., & Sándor, B. (2019).
Embodied robots driven by self-organized environmental feedback.
Adaptive Behavior, 27, 285-294
Laland, K.N. & Galef, B.G. (Eds.). (2009). The question of animal
culture . Harvard University Press.
Laland, K.N. & Williams, K. (1997). Shoaling generates social learning
of foraging information in guppies. Anim. Behav. , 53, 1161–1169.
Leadbeater, E. & Chittka, L. (2007). Social learning in insects - From
miniature brains to consensus building. Curr. Biol. , 17,
703–713.
Leadbeater, E. & Chittka, L. (2009). Bumble-bees learn the value of
social cues through experience. Biol. Lett. , 5, 310–312.
Lee, C., Henshall, J.M., Wark, T.J., Crossman, C.C., Reed, M.T., Brewer,
H.G., et al. (2009). Associative learning by cattle to enable
effective and ethical virtual fences. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. ,
119, 15–22.
Leonard, B. and McNaughton B. (1990). Spatial Representation in the rat:
conceptual, behavioral, and neurophysiological perspectives. In:Neurobiology of Comparative Cognition (ed. Kesner, Raymond P.
Olton, D.S.). Psychology Press, Hillsdale, NJ, p. 488.
Lesort, T., Díaz-Rodríguez, N., Goudou, J.F. and Filliat, D., 2018.
State representation learning for control: An overview. Neural
Networks , 108 , pp.379-392.
Lihoreau, M., Raine, N.E., Reynolds, A.M., Stelzer, R.J., Lim, K.S.,
Smith, A.D., et al. (2012). Radar tracking and motion-sensitive
cameras on flowers reveal the development of pollinator
multi-destination routes over large spatial scales. PLoS Biol. ,
10, 19–21.
Mangel, M. and Clark, C.W., 1988. Dynamic modeling in behavioral
ecology (Vol. 63). Princeton University Press.
Marsh, R., Hao, X., Xu, D., Wang, Z., Duan, Y., Liu, J., et al.(2010). A virtual reality-based FMRI study of reward-based spatial
learning. Neuropsychologia , 48, 2912–2921.
McDonald, W.R., and Cassady St. Clair, C. (2004). The effects of
artificial and natural barriers on the movement of small mammals in
Banff National Park , Canada Oikos , 105, 397–407.
McNamara, J.M. & Houston, A.I. (1985). Optimal foraging and learning.J. Theor. Biol. , 117, 231–249.
Merkle, J.A., Fortin, D. & Morales, J.M. (2014). A memory-based
foraging tactic reveals an adaptive mechanism for restricted space use.Ecol. Lett. , 17, 924–931.
Merkle, J.A., Cherry, S.G., Fortin, D. & Kotler, B.P. (2015). Bison
distribution under conflicting Foraging strategies: Site fidelity vs.
Energy maximization. Ecology , 96, 1793–1801.
Merkle, J.A., Potts, J.R. & Fortin, D. (2017). Energy benefits and
emergent space use patterns of an empirically parameterized model of
memory-based patch selection. Oikos , 126, 185–195.
Merkle, J.A., Sawyer, H., Monteith, K.L., Dwinnell, S.P., Fralick, G.L.
and Kauffman, M.J., 2019. Spatial memory shapes migration and its
benefits: evidence from a large herbivore. Ecology
letters , 22 (11), pp.1797-1805.
Michelot, T., Blackwell, P.G. & Matthiopoulos, J. (2019). Linking
resource selection and step selection models for habitat preferences in
animals. Ecology , 100, 1–12.
Mitchell, M.S. & Powell, R.A. (2004). A mechanistic home range model
for optimal use of spatially distributed resources. Ecol.
Modell. , 177, 209–232.
Moll, R.J., Millspaugh, J.J., Beringer, J., Sartwell, J. and He, Z.,
2007. A new ‘view’of ecology and conservation through animal-borne video
systems. Trends in ecology & evolution , 22 (12),
pp.660-668.
van Moorter, B., Visscher, D., Benhamou, S., Börger, L., Boyce, M.S. &
Gaillard, J.M. (2009). Memory keeps you at home: A mechanistic model for
home range emergence. Oikos , 118, 641–652.
Mora, C. V., Davison, M., Martin Wild, J. & Walker, M.M. (2004).
Magnetoreception and its trigeminal mediation in the homing pigeon.Nature , 432, 508–511.
Morellet, N., Bonenfant, C., Börger, L., Ossi, F., Cagnacci, F.,
Heurich, M., et al. (2013). Seasonality, weather and climate
affect home range size in roe deer across a wide latitudinal gradient
within Europe. J. Anim. Ecol. , 82, 1326–1339.
Mueller, T., Fagan, W.F. & Grimm, V. (2011). Integrating individual
search and navigation behaviors in mechanistic movement models.Theor. Ecol. , 4, 341–355.
Mueller, T., O’Hara, R.B., Converse, S.J., Urbanek, R.P. & Fagan, W.F.
(2013). Social learning of migratory performance. Science , 341,
999–1002.
Murray, M.H., Fassina, S., Hopkins, J.B., Whittington, J. & Clair,
C.C.S. (2017). Seasonal and individual variation in the use of
rail-associated food attractants by grizzly bears (Ursus arctos )
in a national park. PLoS One , 12, 1–16.
Nathan, R. (2008). An emerging movement ecology paradigm.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 105(49),
19050-19051.
Nilsson, J. Å., Brönmark, C., Hansson, L. A., & Chapman, B. B. (2014).
Individuality in movement: the role of animal personality. Animal
movement across scales. L.-A. Hansson and S. Åkesson, eds. pp, 90-109.
Niven, J. E (2016) Neuronal energy consumption: biophysics, efficiency
and evolution. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 41: 129-135.
Noonburg, E.G., Newman, L.A., Lewis, M., Crabtree, R.L. & Potapov, A.B.
(2007). Sequential decision-making in a variable environment: Modeling
elk movement in Yellowstone National Park as a dynamic game.Theor. Popul. Biol. , 71, 182–195.
Owen, C., Rendell, L., Constantine, R., Noad, M.J., Allen, J., Andrews,
O., et al. (2019). Migratory convergence facilitates cultural
transmission of humpback whale song. R. Soc. Open Sci. , 6.
Papastamatiou, Y.P., Cartamil, D.P., Lowe, C.G., Meyer, C.G., Wetherbee,
B.M. & Holland, K.N. (2011). Scales of orientation, directed walks and
movement path structure in sharks. J. Anim. Ecol. , 80, 864–874.
Patrick, S.C.& Weimerskirch, H., 2017. Reproductive success is driven
by local site fidelity despite stronger specialisation by individuals
for large‐scale habitat preference. Journal of Animal
Ecology , 86 (3), pp.674-682.
Pelech, S.A., Smith, J.N.M. & Boutin, S. (2009). A predator’s
perspective of nest predation: predation by red squirrels is learned,
not incidental. Oikos , 841–851.
Polansky, L., W. Kilian, and G. Wittemyer. (2015). Elucidating the
significance of spatial memory on movement decisions by African savannah
elephants using state–space models. Proceedings of the Royal Society B:
Biological Sciences 282: 20143042.
Poor, E. E., Loucks, C., Jakes, A., & Urban, D. L. (2012). Comparing
habitat suitability and connectivity modeling methods for conserving
pronghorn migrations. PLoS One , 7(11).
Potts, J.R. & Lewis, M.A. (2014). How do animal territories form and
change? lessons from 20 years of mechanistic modelling. Proc. R.
Soc. B Biol. Sci. , 281.
Potts, J.R., Mokross, K. & Lewis, M.A. (2014). A unifying framework for
quantifying the nature of animal interactions. J. R. Soc.
Interface , 11.
Preisler, H.K., Ager, A.A. and Wisdom, M.J., 2006. Statistical methods
for analysing responses of wildlife to human disturbance. Journal
of Applied Ecology , 43 (1), pp.164-172.
Reader, S.M. & Biro, D. (2010). Experimental identification of social
learning in wild animals. Learn. Behav. , 38, 265–283.
Rendell, L., Boyd, R., Cownden, D., Enquist, M., Eriksson, K., Feldman,
M.W., et al. (2010). Why Copy Others? Science (80-. ). ,
328, 208–213.
Ronconi, R.A. & Cassady St. Clair, C. (2006). Efficacy of a
radar-activated on-demand system for deterring waterfowl from oil sands
tailings ponds. J. Appl. Ecol. , 43, 111–119.
Scheiner, R. (2004). Responsiveness to sucrose and habituation of the
proboscis extension response in honey bees. J. Comp. Physiol. A
Neuroethol. Sensory, Neural, Behav. Physiol. , 190, 727–733.
Schlägel, U.E. & Lewis, M.A. (2014). Detecting effects of spatial
memory and dynamic information on animal movement decisions.Methods Ecol. Evol. , 5, 1236–1246.
Schlägel, U.E., Merrill, E.H. & Lewis, M.A. (2017). Territory
surveillance and prey management: Wolves keep track of space and time.Ecol. Evol. , 7, 8388–8405.
Scott, R., Marsh, R. & Hays, G.C. (2014). Ontogeny of long distance
migration. Ecology , 95, 2851–5859.
Shettleworth, S.J., Krebs, J.R., Stephens, D.W. and Gibbon, J., 1988.
Tracking a fluctuating environment: a study of sampling. Animal
Behaviour , 36 (1), pp.87-105.
Sigaud, M., Merkle, J.A., Cherry, S.G., Fryxell, J.M., Berdahl, A. &
Fortin, D. (2017). Collective decision-making promotes fitness loss in a
fusion-fission society. Ecol. Lett. , 20, 33–40.
Sih, A. & Mateo, J. (2001). Punishment and persistence pay: A new model
of territory establishment and space use. Trends Ecol. Evol. , 16,
477–479.
Silver, D., Schrittwieser, J., Simonyan, K., Antonoglou, I., Huang, A.,
Guez, A., et al. (2017). Mastering the game of Go without human
knowledge. Nature , 550, 354–359.
Stamps, J.A. (1995). Motor learning and the value of familiar space.Am. Nat. , 146, 41–58.
Stamps, J.A. & Krishnan, V. V. (1999). A Learning-Based Model of
Territory Establishment. Q. Rev. Biol. , 74, 291–318.
Stamps, J.A. & Krishnan, V. V. (2001). How territorial animals compete
for divisible space: A learning-based model with unequal competitors.Am. Nat. , 157, 154–169.
Stanley, D.A., Smith, K.E. & Raine, N.E. (2015). Bumblebee learning and
memory is impaired by chronic exposure to a neonicotinoid pesticide.Sci. Rep. , 5, 1–10.
Stephens, D.W. (1987). On economically tracking a variable environment.Theor. Popul. Biol. , 32, 15–25.
Stephens, D.W., Brown, J.S. & Ydenberg, R.C. (Eds.). (2007).Foraging: behavior and ecology . University of Chicago Press.
Stephens, D.W. & Krebs, J.R. (1987). Foraging Theory . Princeton
University Press.
Sutton, R.S. & Barto, A.G. (Eds.). (2017). Reinforcement
Learning: An Introduction . A Bradford Book.
Tang, W. & Bennett, D.A. (2010). Agent-based modeling of animal
movement: A review. Geogr. Compass , 4, 682–700.
Teitelbaum, C.S., Fagan, W.F., Fleming, C.H., Dressler, G., Calabrese,
J.M., Leimgruber, P., et al. (2015). How far to go? Determinants
of migration distance in land mammals. Ecol. Lett. , 18, 545–552.
Teitelbaum, C.S., Converse, S.J., Fagan, W.F., Böhning-Gaese, K.,
O’Hara, R.B., Lacy, A.E., et al. (2016). Experience drives
innovation of new migration patterns of whooping cranes in response to
global change. Nat. Commun. , 7.
Tello-Ramos, M.C., Hurly, T.A., Higgott, C. & Healy, S.D. (2015).
Time-place learning in wild, free-living hummingbirds. Anim.
Behav. , 104, 123–129.
Tenenbaum, J.B., Griffiths, T.L. & Kemp, C. (2006). Theory-based
Bayesian models of inductive learning and reasoning. Trends Cogn.
Sci. , 10, 309–318.
Thompson, I.D., Bakhtiari, M., Rodgers, A.R., Baker, J.A., Fryxell, J.M.
and Iwachewski, E., 2012. Application of a high‐resolution animal‐borne
remote video camera with global positioning for wildlife study:
Observations on the secret lives of woodland caribou. Wildlife
Society Bulletin , 36 (2), pp.365-370.
Thrun, S. & Pratt, L. (Eds.). (1998). Learning to Learn .
Springer US, Boston, MA.
Viana, D.S., Granados, J.E., Fandos, P., Pérez, J.M., Cano-Manuel, F.J.,
Burón, D., et al. (2018). Linking seasonal home range size with
habitat selection and movement in a mountain ungulate. Mov.
Ecol. , 6, 1–11.
Visalberghi, E. and Fragaszy, D.M., 1990. Do monkeys ape? Pp. 247-273 in
S.T. Parker and K.R. Gibson. Language and intelligence in monkeys and
apes: Comparative developmental perspectives. Cambridge University
Press.
Votier, S.C., Fayet, A.L., Bearhop, S., Bodey, T.W., Clark, B.L.,
Grecian, J., et al. (2017). Effects of age and reproductive
status on individual foraging site fidelity in a long-lived marine
predator. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. , 284, 0–6.
Wakefield, E.D., Furness, R.W., Lane, J. V., Jeglinski, J.W.E. &
Pinder, S.J. (2019). Immature gannets follow adults in commuting flocks
providing a potential mechanism for social learning. J. Avian
Biol. , 1–13.
Wang, Y. S., Blackwell, P. G., Merkle, J. A., & Potts, J. R. (2019).
Continuous time resource selection analysis for moving animals. Methods
in Ecology and Evolution, 10: 1664-1678.
Warner, R.R. (1988). Traditionality of mating-site preferences in a
coral reef fish. Nature , 335, 719–721.
Weinrich, M. (1998). Early experience in habitat choice by humpback
whales (Megaptera novaeanglia e). J. Mammal. , 79, 163–170.
Wolfe, J.L. (1969). Observations on Alertness and Exploratory Behavior
in the Eastern Chipmunk. Am. Midl. Nat. , 81, 249.
Yott, A., Rosatte, R., Schaefer, J.A., Hamr, J. & Fryxell, J. (2011).
Movement and spread of a founding population of reintroduced elk
(Cervus elaphus ) in Ontario, Canada. Restor. Ecol. , 19,
70–77.
Zollner, P.A. and Lima, S.L., 2005. Behavioral tradeoffs when dispersing
across a patchy landscape. Oikos , 108 (2), pp.219-230.