Hypotheses Testing by Axis.
Given that we do not expect to see relationships with stress in the
Placebo group, all following models will be applied on the experimental
group, only (N ≈ 100).
Hypothesis 1: Executive Functioning and Stress Phases.
We will run multilevel models for each stress outcome (STAI, heart rate,
HRV, electrodermal activity, and blood pressure), with executive
functioning facets, time (baseline, instructions, MAST, post-MAST) and
the interaction terms between executive functioning and time as
predictors, along with relevant covariates that correlate with the
dependent and independent variables. We will perform correlations
between all independent variables to optimize model parsimony.
Hypotheses 2, 3, and 4: Mediating roles of Cognitive
Appraisals, Negative Repetitive Thinking, and Emotion Regulation. In
an exploratory manner, we will run partial correlations (Pearson or
Spearman depending on the normality of the distribution) between the
executive functioning facets and all mediators. We will run three types
of multiple mediation models using bootstrapping with Haye’s PROCESS
macro in SPSS Statistics (IBM Corp., 2017), with cognitive appraisals
(four mediators), repetitive negative thinking (two mediators), and
emotion regulation (two mediators). For parsimony reasons, we will
conduct these models when we observe a significant correlation between
(a) an executive functioning facet and one potential mediator, and (b)
one mediator and a stress outcome. In addition, we will include baseline
stress levels for all reactivity models, and the reactivity stress
levels as covariates for the recovery models. This will capture change
from baseline to reactivity, and from reactivity to recovery. We will
report p -values and consider results as significant when they
fulfill the criterion of p < .05.
al’Absi, M. (2018). Stress and Addiction: When a Robust Stress Response
Indicates Resiliency. Psychosomatic Medicine , 80 (1),
2–16. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000520
Barnes, L. L., Harp, D., & Jung, W. S. (2002). Reliability
generalization of scores on the Spielberger state-trait anxiety
inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement ,62 (4), 603–618. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164402062004005
Bassanini, A., Caselli, G., Fiore, F., Ruggiero, G. M., Sassaroli, S.,
& Watkins, E. R. (2014). Why “why” seems better than “how”.
Processes underlining repetitive thinking in an Italian non-clinical
sample. Personality and Individual Differences , 64 ,
18–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.063
Becker, L., Schade, U., & Rohleder, N. (2019). Evaluation of the
socially evaluated cold-pressor group test (SECPT-G) in the general
population. PeerJ , 7 , e7521.
https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.7521/table-3
Bellinghausen, L., Collange, J., Botella, M., Emery, J.-L., & Albert,
É. (2009). Validation factorielle de l’échelle française de stress perçu
en milieu professionnel. Santé Publique , 21 (4), 365–373.
https://doi.org/10.3917/spub.094.0365
Berntson, G. G., Thomas Bigger Jr., J., Eckberg, D. L., Grossman, P.,
Kaufmann, P. G., Malik, M., Nagaraja, H. N., Porges, S. W., Saul, J. P.,
Stone, P. H., & Van Der Molen, M. W. (1997). Heart rate variability:
Origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology ,34 (6), 623–648.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02140.x
Borkovec, T. D., Robinson, E., Pruzinsky, T., & DePree, J. A. (1983).
Preliminary exploration of worry: Some characteristics and processes.Behaviour Research and Therapy , 21 (1), 9–16.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(83)90121-3
Brosschot, J. F. (2010). Markers of chronic stress: Prolonged
physiological activation and (un) conscious perseverative cognition.Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews , 35 (1), 46–50.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.004
Brosschot, J. F., Gerin, W., & Thayer, J. F. (2006). The perseverative
cognition hypothesis: A review of worry, prolonged stress-related
physiological activation, and health. Journal of Psychosomatic
Research , 60 (2), 113–124.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.06.074
Calvo, M. G., & Gutierrez-Garcia, A. (2016). Cognition and stress. InStress: Concepts, cognition, emotion, and behavior (pp.
139–144). Elsevier.
Carpenter, R. (2016). A review of instruments on cognitive appraisal of
stress. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing , 30 (2), 271–279.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apnu.2015.07.002
Christophe, V., Antoine, P., Leroy, T., & Delelis, G. (2009).
Évaluation de deux stratégies de régulation émotionnelle: La suppression
expressive et la réévaluation cognitive. European Review of
Applied Psychology , 59 (1), 59–67.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2008.07.001
Clancy, F., Prestwich, A., Caperon, L., & O’Connor, D. B. (2016).
Perseverative Cognition and Health Behaviors: A Systematic Review and
Meta-Analysis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience , 10 .
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00534
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A Global Measure of
Perceived Stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior ,24 (4), 385. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1994). Perceived stress
scale. Measuring Stress: A Guide for Health and Social
Scientists , 10 , 1–2.
Congdon, E., Mumford, J. A., Cohen, J. R., Galvan, A., Canli, T., &
Poldrack, R. A. (2012). Measurement and reliability of response
inhibition. Frontiers in Psychology , 3 , 37.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00037
Conway, A. R., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm,
O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological
review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review ,12 (5), 769–786. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196772
Cropley, M., Zijlstra, F. R., Querstret, D., & Beck, S. (2016). Is
work-related rumination associated with deficits in executive
functioning? Frontiers in Psychology , 7 , 1524.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01524
Crum, A. J., Jamieson, J. P., & Akinola, M. (2020). Optimizing stress:
An integrated intervention for regulating stress responses.Emotion (Washington, D.C.) , 20 (1), 120–125. Scopus.
https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000670
Davis, R. N., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive inflexibility
among ruminators and nonruminators. Cognitive Therapy and
Research , 24 (6), 699–711.
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005591412406
De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H., Goubert, L., Onraedt, T.,
Vanderhasselt, M.-A., & De Raedt, R. (2012). Cognitive control
moderates the association between stress and rumination. Journal
of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry , 43 (1),
519–525. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.07.004
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of
Psychology , 64 , 135–168.
https://doi.org/10.1146%2Fannurev-psych-113011-143750
Dimitriev, D., & Saperova, E. (2012). Nonlinear parameters of heart
rate variability during mental stress. The FASEB Journal ,26 (S1), lb618–lb618.
https://doi.org/10.1096/fasebj.26.1_supplement.lb618
Ehring, T., & Watkins, E. R. (2008). Repetitive negative thinking as a
transdiagnostic process. International Journal of Cognitive
Therapy , 1 (3), 192–205.
Eslinger, P. J., & Grattan, L. M. (1993). Frontal lobe and
frontal-striatal substrates for different forms of human cognitive
flexibility. Neuropsychologia , 31 (1), 17–28.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(93)90077-D
Everly, G. S., & Lating, J. M. (2002). Measurement of the human
stress response . Springer.
Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Gruen, R. J., & DeLongis, A. (1986).
Appraisal, coping, health status, and psychological symptoms.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 50 (3), 571.
Fox, E., Cahill, S., & Zougkou, K. (2010). Preconscious processing
biases predict emotional reactivity to stress. Biological
Psychiatry , 67 (4), 371–377.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.018
Freeman, D., Bird, J. C., Loe, B. S., Kingdon, D., Startup, H., Clark,
D. M., Ehlers, A., Černis, E., Wingham, G., & Evans, N. (2020). The
Dunn worry questionnaire and the paranoia worries questionnaire: New
assessments of worry. Psychological Medicine , 50 (5),
771–780. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719000588
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R.
P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive
functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General , 137 (2), 201.
https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201
Gaab, J., Rohleder, N., Nater, U. M., & Ehlert, U. (2005).
Psychological determinants of the cortisol stress response: The role of
anticipatory cognitive appraisal. Psychoneuroendocrinology ,30 (6), 599–610. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.02.001
Gauthier, J., & Bouchard, S. (1993). Adaptation canadienne-française de
la forme révisée du State–Trait Anxiety Inventory de Spielberger.Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue Canadienne Des
Sciences Du Comportement , 25 (4), 559.
https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0078881
Gianferante, D., Thoma, M. V., Hanlin, L., Chen, X., Breines, J. G.,
Zoccola, P. M., & Rohleder, N. (2014). Post-stress rumination predicts
HPA axis responses to repeated acute stress.Psychoneuroendocrinology , 49 , 244–252.
Grimm, E. & Agrigoroaei, S. (2023). Executive Functioning as a
Determinant of Examination Stress in University Students: The Mediating
Role of Stress Appraisal, Worry and Coping. In preparation.
Grimm, E., Agrigoroaei, S., Rohleder, N., & Becker, L. (2021).
Executive functioning as a predictor of physiological and subjective
acute stress responses in non-clinical adult populations: A systematic
literature review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
Reviews .https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.09.037
Gronwall, D. M. A. (1977). Paced auditory serial-addition task: A
measure of recovery from concussion. Perceptual and Motor Skills ,44 (2), 367–373. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1977.44.2.367
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An
integrative review. Review of General Psychology , 2 (3),
271–299. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
Gross, J. J. (2014). Emotion regulation: Conceptual and empirical
foundations. In Handbook of emotion regulation, 2nd ed (pp.
3–20). The Guilford Press.
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two
emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships,
and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,85 , 348–362. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348
Gustavson, D. E., du Pont, A., Whisman, M. A., & Miyake, A. (2018).
Evidence for Transdiagnostic Repetitive Negative Thinking and Its
Association with Rumination, Worry, and Depression and Anxiety Symptoms:
A Commonality Analysis. Collabra: Psychology , 4 (1), 13.
https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.128
Harvey, A. G., Watkins, E., & Mansell, W. (2004). Cognitive
behavioural processes across psychological disorders: A transdiagnostic
approach to research and treatment . Oxford University Press, USA.
Hendricks, M. A., & Buchanan, T. W. (2016). Individual differences in
cognitive control processes and their relationship to emotion
regulation. Cognition and Emotion , 30 (5), 912–924.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1032893
Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive
functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences ,16 (3), 174–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006
Inquisit 6 [Computer software]. (2021). Retrieved from
https://www.millisecond.com.
Jennings, J. R., Bberg, W. K., Hutcheson, J. S., Obrist, P., Porges, S.,
& Turpin, G. (1981). Publication guidelines for heart rate studies in
man. Psychophysiology , 18 , 226–231.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb03023.x
Johnson, D. R. (2009). Emotional attention set-shifting and its
relationship to anxiety and emotion regulation. Emotion ,9 , 681–690. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017095
Joormann, J., Levens, S. M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2011). Sticky Thoughts:
Depression and Rumination Are Associated With Difficulties Manipulating
Emotional Material in Working Memory. Psychological Science ,22 (8), 979–983. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611415539
Kan, Y., Xue, W., Zhao, H., Wang, X., Guo, X., & Duan, H. (2021). The
discrepant effect of acute stress on cognitive inhibition and response
inhibition. Consciousness and Cognition , 91 , 103131.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2021.103131
Kim, H.-G., Cheon, E.-J., Bai, D.-S., Lee, Y. H., & Koo, B.-H. (2018).
Stress and Heart Rate Variability: A Meta-Analysis and Review of the
Literature. Psychiatry Investigation , 15 (3), 235–245.
https://doi.org/10.30773/pi.2017.08.17
Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K.-M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The ‘Trier
Social Stress Test’ – A Tool for Investigating Psychobiological Stress
Responses in a Laboratory Setting. Neuropsychobiology ,28 (1–2), 76–81. https://doi.org/10.1159/000119004
Laborde, S., Mosley, E., & Thayer, J. F. (2017). Heart Rate Variability
and Cardiac Vagal Tone in Psychophysiological Research –
Recommendations for Experiment Planning, Data Analysis, and Data
Reporting. Frontiers in Psychology , 8 .
https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00213
Labuschagne, I., Grace, C., Rendell, P., Terrett, G., & Heinrichs, M.
(2019). An introductory guide to conducting the Trier Social Stress
Test. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews , 107 ,
686–695. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.09.032
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and
coping . Springer publishing company.
Lesage, F.-X., Berjot, S., & Deschamps, F. (2012). Clinical stress
assessment using a visual analogue scale. Occupational Medicine ,62 (8), 600–605. https://doi.org/10.1093/occmed/kqs140
Levinson, D. F., & Edelberg, R. (1985). Scoring Criteria for Response
Latency and Habituation in Electrodermal Research: A Critique.Psychophysiology , 22 (4), 417–426.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1985.tb01626.x
Luettgau, L., Schlagenhauf, F., & Sjoerds, Z. (2018). Acute and past
subjective stress influence working memory and related neural
substrates. Psychoneuroendocrinology , 96 , 25–34.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.05.036
Maeda, S., Sato, T., Shimada, H., & Tsumura, H. (2017). Post-event
Processing Predicts Impaired Cortisol Recovery Following Social
Stressor: The Moderating Role of Social Anxiety. Frontiers in
Psychology , 8 .
https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01919
Malik, M. (1996). Heart Rate Variability. Annals of Noninvasive
Electrocardiology , 1 (2), 151–181.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1542-474X.1996.tb00275.x
McLaughlin, K. A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011). Rumination as a
transdiagnostic factor in depression and anxiety. Behaviour
Research and Therapy , 49 (3), 186–193.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.12.006
McRae, K., & Gross, J. J. (2020). Emotion regulation. Emotion
(Washington, D.C.) , 20 (1), 1–9.
https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000703
Meiran, N., Diamond, G. M., Toder, D., & Nemets, B. (2011). Cognitive
rigidity in unipolar depression and obsessive compulsive disorder:
Examination of task switching, Stroop, working memory updating and
post-conflict adaptation. Psychiatry Research , 185 (1),
149–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.04.044
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The Nature and Organization of
Individual Differences in Executive Functions: Four General Conclusions.Current Directions in Psychological Science , 21 (1), 8–14.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter,
A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The Unity and Diversity of Executive
Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A
Latent Variable Analysis. Cognitive Psychology , 41 (1),
49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
Moberly, N. J., & Watkins, E. R. (2006). Processing Mode Influences the
Relationship Between Trait Rumination and Emotional Vulnerability.Behavior Therapy , 37 (3), 281–291.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006.02.003
Myruski, S., Denefrio, S., & Dennis-Tiwary, T. A. (2018). Stress and
Emotion Regulation: The Dynamic Fit Model. In The Oxford Handbook
of Stress and Mental Health (Kate Harkness and Elizabeth P. Hayden, p.
415). Oxford University Press.
Narvaez Linares, N. F., Charron, V., Ouimet, A. J., Labelle, P. R., &
Plamondon, H. (2020). A systematic review of the Trier Social Stress
Test methodology: Issues in promoting study comparison and replicable
research. Neurobiology of Stress , 13 , 100235.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ynstr.2020.100235
Nater, U. M., & Rohleder, N. (2009). Salivary alpha-amylase as a
non-invasive biomarker for the sympathetic nervous system: Current state
of research. Psychoneuroendocrinology , 34 (4), 486–496.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.01.014
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking
Rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science , 3 (5),
400–424. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x
Ottaviani, C. (2018). Brain-heart interaction in perseverative
cognition. Psychophysiology , 55 (7), e13082.
https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13082
Ottaviani, C., Thayer, J. F., Verkuil, B., Lonigro, A., Medea, B.,
Couyoumdjian, A., & Brosschot, J. F. (2016). Physiological concomitants
of perseverative cognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis.Psychological Bulletin , 142 , 231–259.
https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000036
Packwood, S., Hodgetts, H. M., & Tremblay, S. (2011). A
multiperspective approach to the conceptualization of executive
functions. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology ,33 (4), 456–470. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2010.533157
Panaite, V., Salomon, K., Jin, A., & Rottenberg, J. (2015).
Cardiovascular recovery from psychological and physiological challenge
and risk for adverse cardiovascular outcomes and all-cause mortality.Psychosomatic Medicine , 77 (3), 215–226.
https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000171
Panganiban, A. R., & Matthews, G. (2014). Executive Functioning
Protects Against Stress in UAV Simulation. Proceedings of the
Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting , 58 (1),
994–998. https://doi.org/10.1177/1541931214581208
Paszkiel, S., Dobrakowski, P., & Łysiak, A. (2020). The Impact of
Different Sounds on Stress Level in the Context of EEG, Cardiac Measures
and Subjective Stress Level: A Pilot Study. Brain Sciences ,10 (10), 10. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10100728
Philippot, P. (2019). The State Repetitive Thinking Questionnaire[Internal Report]. Laboratory for Experimental Psychopathology,
UCLouvain, Belgium.
Pruessner, L., Barnow, S., Holt, D. V., Joormann, J., & Schulze, K.
(2020). A cognitive control framework for understanding emotion
regulation flexibility. Emotion , 20 , 21–29.
https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000658
Quintana, D. S., Alvares, G. A., & Heathers, J. a. J. (2016).
Guidelines for Reporting Articles on Psychiatry and Heart rate
variability (GRAPH): Recommendations to advance research communication.Translational Psychiatry , 6 (5), 5.
https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2016.73
Reyes del Paso, G. A., Langewitz, W., Mulder, L. J. M., van Roon, A., &
Duschek, S. (2013). The utility of low frequency heart rate variability
as an index of sympathetic cardiac tone: A review with emphasis on a
reanalysis of previous studies. Psychophysiology , 50 (5),
477–487. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12027
Rocha-Oliveira, D., & Zibetti, M. R. (2022). Impact of repetitive
negative thinking on reactivity and recovery from physiological stress
in clinical and non-clinical individuals. Journal of Affective
Disorders Reports , 8 , 100338.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadr.2022.100338
Rodríguez-Nieto, G., Seer, C., Sidlauskaite, J., Vleugels, L., Van Roy,
A., Hardwick, R., & Swinnen, S. (2022). Inhibition, Shifting and
Updating: Inter and intra-domain commonalities and differences from an
executive functions activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis.NeuroImage , 264 , 119665.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119665
Schmeichel, B. J., & Tang, D. (2015). Individual Differences in
Executive Functioning and Their Relationship to Emotional Processes and
Responses. Current Directions in Psychological Science ,24 (2), 93–98. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414555178
Schwabe, L., & Schächinger, H. (2018). Ten years of research with the
Socially Evaluated Cold Pressor Test: Data from the past and guidelines
for the future. Psychoneuroendocrinology , 92 , 155–161.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.03.010
Schwerdtfeger, A., & Derakshan, N. (2010). The time line of threat
processing and vagal withdrawal in response to a self-threatening
stressor in cognitive avoidant copers: Evidence for vigilance-avoidance
theory. Psychophysiology , 47 (4), 786–795.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00965.x
Segerstrom, S. C., Stanton, A. L., Alden, L. E., & Shortridge, B. E.
(2003). A Multidimensional Structure for Repetitive Thought: What’s on
Your Mind, and How, and How Much? Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology , 85 , 909–921.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.909
Setz, C., Arnrich, B., Schumm, J., La Marca, R., Tröster, G., & Ehlert,
U. (2010). Discriminating Stress From Cognitive Load Using a Wearable
EDA Device. IEEE Transactions on Information Technology in
Biomedicine , 14 (2), 410–417.
https://doi.org/10.1109/TITB.2009.2036164
Shapiro, D., Jamner, L. D., Lane, J. D., Light, K. C., Myrtek, M.,
Sawada, Y., & Steptoe, A. (1996). Blood pressure publication
guidelines. Psychophysiology , 33 (1), 1–12.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb02103.x
Shields, G. S., Sazma, M. A., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2016). The effects of
acute stress on core executive functions: A meta-analysis and comparison
with cortisol. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews , 68 ,
651–668. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.038
Shilton, A. L., Laycock, R., & Crewther, S. G. (2017). The Maastricht
Acute Stress Test (MAST): Physiological and Subjective Responses in
Anticipation, and Post-stress. Frontiers in Psychology , 8 .
https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00567
Smeets, T., Cornelisse, S., Quaedflieg, C. W. E. M., Meyer, T., Jelicic,
M., & Merckelbach, H. (2012). Introducing the Maastricht Acute Stress
Test (MAST): A quick and non-invasive approach to elicit robust
autonomic and glucocorticoid stress responses.Psychoneuroendocrinology , 37 (12), 1998–2008.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.04.012
Society for Psychophysiological Research Ad Hoc Committee on
Electrodermal Measures. (2012). Publication recommendations for
electrodermal measurements. Psychophysiology , 49 (8),
1017–1034. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01384.x
Spielberger, C. D. (1983). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Adults.Palo Alto (CA): Consulting Psychologists Press .
https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/t06496-000
Stone, C., Ney, L., Felmingham, K., Nichols, D., & Matthews, A. (2021).
The effects of acute stress on attentional networks and working memory
in females. Physiology & Behavior , 113602.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113602
Tanner, A., Voon, D., Hasking, P., & Martin, G. (2013). Underlying
Structure of Ruminative Thinking: Factor Analysis of the Ruminative
Thought Style Questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research ,37 (3), 633–646. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-012-9492-1
Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American
Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. (1996). Heart rate variability:
Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical
use. Circulation , 93 (5), 1043–1065.
Thayer, J. F., & Lane, R. D. (2000). A model of neurovisceral
integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. Journal of
Affective Disorders , 61 (3), 201–216.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00338-4
Topouchian, J., Agnoletti, D., Blacher1, J., Youssef,
A., Chahine, M. N., Ibanez, I., Assemani, N., & Asmar, R. (2014).
Validation of four devices: Omron M6 Comfort, Omron HEM-7420, Withings
BP-800, and Polygreen KP-7670 for home blood pressure measurement
according to the European Society of Hypertension International
Protocol. Vascular Health and Risk Management , 10 , 33–44.
https://doi.org/10.2147/VHRM.S53968
Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2005). An
automated version of the operation span task. Behavior Research
Methods , 37 (3), 498–505. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192720
Verbruggen, F., Aron, A. R., Band, G. P., Beste, C., Bissett, P. G.,
Brockett, A. T., Brown, J. W., Chamberlain, S. R., Chambers, C. D.,
Colonius, H., Colzato, L. S., Corneil, B. D., Coxon, J. P., Dupuis, A.,
Eagle, D. M., Garavan, H., Greenhouse, I., Heathcote, A., Huster, R. J.,
… Boehler, C. N. (2019). A consensus guide to capturing the
ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal
task. ELife , 8 , e46323.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46323
Verbruggen, F., Logan, G. D., & Stevens, M. A. (2008). STOP-IT: Windows
executable software for the stop-signal paradigm. Behavior
Research Methods , 40 (2), 479–483.
https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.2.479
Verkuil, B., Brosschot, J. F., Gebhardt, W. A., & Thayer, J. F. (2010).
When Worries Make you Sick: A Review of Perseverative Cognition, the
Default Stress Response and Somatic Health. Journal of
Experimental Psychopathology , 1 (1), jep.009110.
https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.009110
Vila, X. A., Lado, M. J., & Cuesta-Morales, P. (2019). Evidence Based
Recommendations for Designing Heart Rate Variability Studies.Journal of Medical Systems , 43 (10), 1–10.
Watkins, E., Moberly, N. J., & Moulds, M. L. (2008). Processing Mode
Causally Influences Emotional Reactivity. Emotion (Washington,
D.C.) , 8 (3), 364–378. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.364
Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive
thought. Psychological Bulletin , 134 (2), 163–206.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163
Watkins, E. R., & Roberts, H. (2020). Reflecting on rumination:
Consequences, causes, mechanisms and treatment of rumination.Behaviour Research and Therapy , 127 .
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2020.103573
Webb, T. L., Miles, E., & Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: A
meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the
process model of emotion regulation. Psychological Bulletin ,138 , 775–808. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027600
Whitmer, A. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2013). An attentional scope model of
rumination. Psychological Bulletin , 139 , 1036–1061.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030923
Wirtz, P. H., Ehlert, U., Emini, L., Rüdisüli, K., Groessbauer, S.,
Gaab, J., Elsenbruch, S., & von Känel, R. (2006). Anticipatory
Cognitive Stress Appraisal and the Acute Procoagulant Stress Response in
Men. Psychosomatic Medicine , 68 (6), 851.
https://doi.org/10.1097/01.psy.0000245866.03456.aa
Xin, Y., Yao, Z., Wang, W., Luo, Y., Aleman, A., & Wu, J. (2020).
Recent life stress predicts blunted acute stress response and the role
of executive control. Stress , 23 (3), 359–367.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890.2019.1687684
Yang, Y., Cao, S., Shields, G. S., Teng, Z., & Liu, Y. (2017). The
relationships between rumination and core executive functions: A
meta-analysis. Depression and Anxiety , 34 (1), 37–50.
https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22539
Zandara, M., Garcia-Lluch, M., Pulopulos, M. M., Hidalgo, V., Villada,
C., & Salvador, A. (2016). Acute stress and working memory: The role of
sex and cognitive stress appraisal. Physiology & Behavior ,164 , 336–344. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.06.022
Zelazo, P. D., & Cunningham, W. A. (2007). Executive Function:
Mechanisms Underlying Emotion Regulation. In Handbook of emotion
regulation (pp. 135–158). The Guilford Press.
Zoccola, P. M., & Dickerson, S. S. (2015). Extending the recovery
window: Effects of trait rumination on subsequent evening cortisol
following a laboratory performance stressor.Psychoneuroendocrinology , 58 , 67–78.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.04.014